Preface |
|
v | |
Program Committee |
|
vii | |
|
|
1 | (22) |
|
|
3 | (20) |
|
|
6 | (1) |
|
|
7 | (1) |
|
|
8 | (1) |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
|
10 | (1) |
|
|
11 | (1) |
|
|
12 | (1) |
|
|
13 | (1) |
|
|
14 | (1) |
|
|
15 | (1) |
|
|
16 | (1) |
|
|
17 | (1) |
|
|
18 | (1) |
|
|
19 | (1) |
|
|
20 | (1) |
|
|
21 | (1) |
|
|
22 | (1) |
|
|
23 | (346) |
|
Diachronic processes in language as signaling under conflicting interests |
|
|
25 | (8) |
|
|
|
Syntactic development in phenotypic space |
|
|
33 | (8) |
|
|
|
Finding the underpinnings: The last quarter century |
|
|
41 | (9) |
|
|
Strategies for the emergence of first-order phrase structure |
|
|
50 | (8) |
|
|
|
What were we talking about? Exchanging social models as a route to language |
|
|
58 | (8) |
|
|
Why might SOV be initially preferred and then lost or recovered? A theoretical framework |
|
|
66 | (8) |
|
|
Linguistic animals: Understanding language through a comparative approach |
|
|
74 | (8) |
|
|
Creative compositionality from reinforcement learning in signaling games |
|
|
82 | (8) |
|
|
Overlapping and synchronization in the song of the Indris (Indri indri) |
|
|
90 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
A matter of perspective: Viewpoint phenomena in the evolution of grammar |
|
|
98 | (8) |
|
|
|
A constructionist approach to the evolution of morphological complexity |
|
|
106 | (8) |
|
|
Language evolved for storytelling in a super-fast evolution |
|
|
114 | (8) |
|
Till Nikolaus von Heiseler |
|
|
What iconicity can and cannot do for proto-Language |
|
|
122 | (8) |
|
|
Did language evolve incommunicado? |
|
|
130 | (8) |
|
|
Hunter-gatherer egalitarianism enabled grammar to evolve |
|
|
138 | (8) |
|
|
|
Grasping compositional patterns in an artificial language by Chinese participants |
|
|
146 | (8) |
|
|
|
The emergence of compound signals |
|
|
154 | (7) |
|
|
|
Modality switch in human language evolution |
|
|
161 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Broadcasting to the enemy: Deception as a solution in evolution of language |
|
|
169 | (8) |
|
|
Recursion is not language domain-specific: Interim results of a research program |
|
|
177 | (8) |
|
|
On the emergence of bilingualism in a communication "ALL" task as a result of competition between social conformism and language simplification |
|
|
185 | (8) |
|
|
Establishing a communication system: Miscommunication drives abstraction |
|
|
193 | (2) |
|
|
On the reliability of unreliable information: Gossip as cultural memory |
|
|
195 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
197 | (8) |
|
|
The phonatory culture hypothesis |
|
|
205 | (8) |
|
|
Evolution of tense and aspect |
|
|
213 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Orofacial gestures in language evolution: The auditory feedback hypothesis |
|
|
221 | (7) |
|
|
|
|
Iconicity and ape gesture |
|
|
228 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Iterative vocal charades: The emergence of conventions in vocal communication |
|
|
236 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Constructions, construal and cooperation in the evolution of language |
|
|
244 | (8) |
|
|
|
Female philopatry and egalitarianism as conditions for the emergence of intersubjectivity |
|
|
252 | (8) |
|
|
The role of coordination in regularization |
|
|
260 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The psychology of biological clocks: A new framework for the evolution of rhythm |
|
|
262 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
The paradox of linguistic complexity and community size |
|
|
270 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Social interaction influences the evolution of cognitive biases for language |
|
|
278 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Understanding the linguistic structure and evolution of web search queries |
|
|
286 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
The role of iconicity in the evolution of linguistic structure |
|
|
294 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Linearisation of adjectives: The grammatical face of perceptual/conceptual biases? |
|
|
296 | (6) |
|
|
Supporting evidence for language polygenesis from Neanderthal-Human interbreeding |
|
|
302 | (8) |
|
|
Language emergence in the laboratory: A method suitable to dynamical systems analysis |
|
|
310 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Is the syntax rubicon more of a mirage? |
|
|
318 | (8) |
|
|
Symbol extension and meaning generation in cultural evolution for displaced communication |
|
|
326 | (8) |
|
|
|
Fitness landscapes in cultural language evolution: A case study on German definite articles |
|
|
334 | (8) |
|
|
Social word learning strategies in different cultures |
|
|
342 | (2) |
|
|
|
The mental synthesis theory: The dual origin of human language |
|
|
344 | (9) |
|
|
Cognitive factors motivating the evolution of word meanings: Evidence from corpora, behavioral data and encyclopedic network structure |
|
|
353 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
The magic number 4: Evolutionary pressures on semantic frame structure |
|
|
361 | (8) |
|
|
|
369 | (198) |
|
Rule learning in humans and animals |
|
|
371 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The Putty-nosed monkey `Pyow-Hack' sequence: Compositional or an idiomatic expression? |
|
|
373 | (2) |
|
|
|
Primate pragmatics: Putty-nosed monkeys use contextual information to disambiguate the cause of alarm calls |
|
|
375 | (2) |
|
|
|
The evolution of human cognitive systems: Comparative approaches to language and music |
|
|
377 | (2) |
|
|
|
Sociocultural determiners of linguistic complexity |
|
|
379 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Speaking of language and evolution |
|
|
381 | (2) |
|
|
Language disorders as windows on language evolution |
|
|
383 | (2) |
|
|
|
Zipf's law across languages of the world: Towards a quantitative measure of lexical diversity |
|
|
385 | (2) |
|
|
|
Informational structure of an emerging communication system is shaped by its environment |
|
|
387 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Spirals in language evolution |
|
|
389 | (2) |
|
|
Sound symbolism and the origins of language |
|
|
391 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
The origins of combinatorial communication |
|
|
393 | (2) |
|
|
|
A proposal concerning the gene network that regulates the shape of the language-ready brain |
|
|
395 | (2) |
|
|
|
Sign-theory and the origin of language |
|
|
397 | (2) |
|
|
Social origins of rhythm? Synchrony and temporal regularity in human vocalization |
|
|
399 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Bridging the gap: From bodily mimesis to speech |
|
|
401 | (2) |
|
|
|
The emergence of combinatoriality in the cultural transmission of pop songs in a children's gameshow |
|
|
403 | (2) |
|
|
The cumulative cultural evolution of category structure in an open-ended meaning space |
|
|
405 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Do talk to strangers: Maternal and non-maternal interaction in the transmission of primate gesture |
|
|
407 | (2) |
|
|
|
The evolution of polysemy in child language |
|
|
409 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Zebra finches can learn to recognize affixations |
|
|
411 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Vocal communication in Gibbons |
|
|
413 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The dissolution of language & speech following brain damage |
|
|
415 | (2) |
|
|
Frequency and stability of linguistic variants |
|
|
417 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Biological adaptation to cultural traits |
|
|
419 | (2) |
|
|
Language and speech are old: A review of the evidence and consequences for modern linguistic diversity |
|
|
421 | (2) |
|
|
|
The role of the human political singularity in the emergence of language |
|
|
423 | (2) |
|
|
Conversational infrastructure and the convergent evolution of linguistic items |
|
|
425 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Words as unmotivated cues |
|
|
427 | (2) |
|
|
|
Representations are selected: They don't just drift |
|
|
429 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
The cumulative cultural evolution of an instruction language |
|
|
431 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Birds tutored with their own developing song produce wildtype-like song as adults |
|
|
433 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Regularization in language evolution: On the joint contribution of domain-specific biases and domain-general frequency learning |
|
|
435 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The effect of pitch enhancement on spoken language acquisition |
|
|
437 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Language from gesture? Emergent transitivity marking in Nicaraguan Sign Language |
|
|
439 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Four wrong ideas in evolutionary linguistics |
|
|
441 | (2) |
|
|
Artificial grammar learning in infants, adults, and songbirds: What is shared, what is learned? |
|
|
443 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
A revival of the homo loquens as a builder of labeled structures |
|
|
445 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Language development in children with laryngeal abnormalities identifies prerequisites for verbal protolanguage |
|
|
447 | (2) |
|
|
|
Multimodal communication in wild chimpanzees |
|
|
449 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Comparative method for determining lexical stress in nonsense words |
|
|
451 | (2) |
|
|
|
Sound symbolism and arbitrary sound-meaning relationships in language |
|
|
453 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Tracing language primitives: Phonosemantic realization of fundamental oppositional pairs |
|
|
455 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
The origins of regularity in language: Why coordination matters |
|
|
457 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Efficient communication and language evolution |
|
|
459 | (2) |
|
|
Evolutionary paths to compositional language |
|
|
461 | (2) |
|
|
|
Systems emerge: The cultural evolution of interdependent sequential behaviours in the lab |
|
|
463 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Formant tuning technique in vocalizations of non-human primates |
|
|
465 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Bow-and-arrow technology: Mapping human cognition and perhaps language evolution |
|
|
467 | (2) |
|
Alexandra Regina Kratschmer |
|
|
|
|
Patterns of variation in language and tool use: An ethnographic and comparative approach |
|
|
469 | (2) |
|
|
From grasping to grooming to gossip |
|
|
471 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
A multimodal perspective on ape communication |
|
|
473 | (2) |
|
|
Social structure from language games |
|
|
475 | (2) |
|
|
|
Getting communication started: The superiority of gesture over non-linguistic vocalization |
|
|
477 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
The effect of size of articulation space on the emergence of combinatorial structure |
|
|
479 | (2) |
|
|
|
Comparative psychology and the evolution of language: Methodology matters |
|
|
481 | (2) |
|
|
Pronomial characteristics of an evolved language: Is brevity an evolutionary advantage? |
|
|
483 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Culture vs. biology: Adversarial coevolution during the evolution of the lexicon |
|
|
485 | (2) |
|
|
From hand to mouth: Fine precision grip during mutual grooming elicited wide lip movements in wild Fongoli chimpanzees |
|
|
487 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
The nature of language in interaction |
|
|
489 | (2) |
|
|
Dogs need embodied directions: Children but not dogs possess skills needed for communicating with absent interlocutors |
|
|
491 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Is Gricean communication necessarily cooperative? |
|
|
493 | (2) |
|
|
What Dwight L. Bolinger probably would have contributed to evolutionary linguistics |
|
|
495 | (2) |
|
|
Motivated vs. conventional systematicity: Implications for language learning and the structure of the lexicon |
|
|
497 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The role of vocal learning in the acoustic communication of the Egyptian fruit bat |
|
|
499 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Detecting differences between the languages of Neandertals and modern humans |
|
|
501 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The effect of iconicity on the emergence of combinatorial structure: An experimental study |
|
|
503 | (2) |
|
|
|
Accelerated regions and the language faculty |
|
|
505 | (2) |
|
|
Chimpanzee food grunts are directed at specific individuals: Precursors for triadic communication? |
|
|
507 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
About time: Semantic structure in emerging language |
|
|
509 | (2) |
|
|
Handicaps are unnecessary for human communication |
|
|
511 | (2) |
|
|
Maxwell N. Burton-Chellew |
|
|
|
The origins of word meaning |
|
|
513 | (2) |
|
|
Intentionality in the production of chimpanzee alarm calls |
|
|
515 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
The cognitive underpinnings of metaphor as the driving force of language evolution |
|
|
517 | (2) |
|
|
|
Prosodic cue weighting by zebra finches |
|
|
519 | (2) |
|
|
|
Minimal requirements for the emergence of learned signalling |
|
|
521 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Incremental recruitment language---A formalism for evolutionary semantics |
|
|
523 | (2) |
|
|
Momentum-based language change: A non-adaptive model of directional selection |
|
|
525 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Symbolisation and cognition |
|
|
527 | (2) |
|
|
The evolutionary relations between music and language: A cross-musical idiom approach from the comparative perspective of language and music |
|
|
529 | (2) |
|
|
|
Organization of language: Evaluation of modularity theories |
|
|
531 | (2) |
|
|
|
Culture: Copying, compression and conventionality |
|
|
533 | (2) |
|
|
|
Model fitting and prediction for language evolution |
|
|
535 | (2) |
|
|
|
The effect of communication on category structure |
|
|
537 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
On the relations between articulatory gestures and manual grasping |
|
|
539 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Learning speech-like signals from a skewed continuous distribution |
|
|
541 | (2) |
|
|
|
Development of language through shared intentionality and categorization |
|
|
543 | (2) |
|
|
Iterated learning of sound systems and the emergence of tone categories |
|
|
545 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
547 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Frequency-dependent bias affects the spread of human communication systems |
|
|
549 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Adaptive strategies in the origins of semantic categories |
|
|
551 | (2) |
|
|
The influence of music on the perception of emotions in voice samples: Evolutionary implications |
|
|
553 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Semantic crowding triggers systematically structured sign systems |
|
|
555 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Speech is characterized by robustness, neutrality and evolvability |
|
|
557 | (2) |
|
|
Experimentally investigating the role of context in the structuring of the linguistic system over cultural evolution |
|
|
559 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Neural networks, algebraic rules & human uniqueness |
|
|
561 | (2) |
|
|
|
Modelling language competition without prestige |
|
|
563 | (2) |
|
|
|
Requirements on scenarios for the evolution of language and cognition |
|
|
565 | (2) |
|
Authors Index |
|
567 | |