Preface |
|
v | |
Panel of Reviewers |
|
vii | |
|
|
|
Evolutionary parallels between language and tool use |
|
|
3 | (8) |
|
|
Cortico-cortical and cortico-cerebellar computations in language change |
|
|
11 | (8) |
|
|
Towards language acquisition by cognitive developmental robotics |
|
|
19 | (7) |
|
|
Protolanguage, discrete infinity and interfaces: Investigating the evolution of the language and music faculty within the minimalist program |
|
|
26 | (8) |
|
|
Language and friendships: A co-evolution model of social and linguistic conventions |
|
|
34 | (8) |
|
|
|
Campbell's monkeys alarm calls are not morpheme-based |
|
|
42 | (8) |
|
|
|
On the inference `Neanderthals had FOXP2 = they had complex language' |
|
|
50 | (8) |
|
|
|
The impact of L2 speakers on the evolution of case marking |
|
|
58 | (6) |
|
|
|
Introducing pressure for expressivity into language evolution experiments |
|
|
64 | (8) |
|
|
The evolution of the Greenbergian word order correlations |
|
|
72 | (8) |
|
|
Semiotic investigations into early forms of symbolism and language |
|
|
80 | (8) |
|
|
Sea crossings are an unreliable indicator of language ability in hominids |
|
|
88 | (8) |
|
|
|
Overgeneralization of verbs --- The change of the German verb system |
|
|
96 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
Was the introduction of writing an evolutionary transition? |
|
|
104 | (6) |
|
|
Medium and long-run properties of linguistic community evolution |
|
|
110 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Scales, salience and referential safety: The benefit of communicating the extreme |
|
|
118 | (8) |
|
|
Non-human primates cannot decontextualize and objectify the actions of their conspecifics |
|
|
126 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
How did predication evolve? |
|
|
134 | (8) |
|
|
Individual and social effects on linguistic diffusion |
|
|
142 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Ontogeny of two communicative tools: Distance encoding & multimodality in deictic pointing |
|
|
150 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Speech development in previously aphonic children after airway reconstruction recapitulates evolution of spoken language |
|
|
158 | (7) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
165 | (8) |
|
|
The case for Neanderthal language -- How strong is it? |
|
|
173 | (8) |
|
|
Meanings of touching object parts in pointing |
|
|
181 | (8) |
|
|
|
The evolution of morphology and the diversity of grammatical systems |
|
|
189 | (7) |
|
|
What's the scope of the Naming Game? Constraints on semantic categorization |
|
|
196 | (8) |
|
|
|
The origins of linguistic predicate/argument structure |
|
|
204 | (8) |
|
|
The voice of things: The revolution of human language and its origin from sound imitation |
|
|
212 | (7) |
|
|
Empirical approaches to recursion |
|
|
219 | (8) |
|
|
|
Outgroup: The study of chimpanzees to know the human mind |
|
|
227 | (7) |
|
|
A modulation-demodulation model for speech communication and its emergence |
|
|
234 | (8) |
|
|
Signaling conventions: Who learns what where and when in a social network? |
|
|
242 | (8) |
|
|
|
Hominin cooperation and language evolution |
|
|
250 | (8) |
|
|
Morphing social communication networks from chimpanzee to human type |
|
|
258 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Gesture and the origins of language |
|
|
266 | (8) |
|
|
Ambiguity resolution and evolution of word order |
|
|
274 | (8) |
|
|
|
When recursion collapses: Evidence from discourse interaction |
|
|
282 | (6) |
|
|
Cognitive construal, mental spaces and the evolution of language and cognition |
|
|
288 | (8) |
|
|
Spatial dynamics of language |
|
|
296 | (8) |
|
|
Are web search queries an evolving protolanguage? |
|
|
304 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Language change in socially structured populations |
|
|
312 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
Language lateralization, categorical perception and language evolution |
|
|
320 | (8) |
|
|
|
Potential stages in the cultural evolution of spatial language |
|
|
328 | (8) |
|
|
Visual artificial grammar learning: Comparative research on humans and birds |
|
|
336 | (8) |
|
|
|
Syntax of mind -- Semantics of mind: Two Frameworks for comparative approaches to the evolution of language and music |
|
|
344 | (8) |
|
|
|
Displacement in communication |
|
|
352 | (8) |
|
|
|
The emergence of morphosyntactic case systems |
|
|
360 | (8) |
|
|
Holistic or synthetic protolanguage: Evidence from iterated learning of whistled signals |
|
|
368 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
The information rate of modern speech and its implications for language evolution |
|
|
376 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Robustness as a design feature of speech communication |
|
|
384 | (8) |
|
|
|
The cooperative nature of conversation: Evidence from conversational exchanges |
|
|
392 | (11) |
|
|
|
|
|
Postnatal acquirement of the ability to discriminate culturaly shared song syntax in songbirds |
|
|
403 | (2) |
|
|
A simple integrated framework for investigating genetic and cultural evolution of language |
|
|
405 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
An adaptationist approach to the audience design hypothesis |
|
|
407 | (2) |
|
|
The the exponent of Zipf's law in language ontogeny |
|
|
409 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Reconciling the diversity of languages with the biological uniformity of their speakers |
|
|
411 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
413 | (3) |
|
|
The effects of modified variables on an iterated learning model of linguistic evolution by cultural transmission |
|
|
416 | (2) |
|
|
Is pantomime a likely stage in language evolution? Evidence from human and primate gesture |
|
|
418 | (2) |
|
|
|
Is And a fossil?: Coordination and the origin of compositionality |
|
|
420 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
The first word was not a noun |
|
|
422 | (2) |
|
|
Iconicity in structured form and meaning spaces |
|
|
424 | (2) |
|
|
Neither nature nor nurture: Coevolution, devolution, and universality of language |
|
|
426 | (2) |
|
|
Comparative histological data between the vocal folds of humans and bonobo |
|
|
428 | (2) |
|
|
|
On some parallels between the vocal apparatus and musical instruments, and their consequences for the evolution of language and music |
|
|
430 | (2) |
|
Jose Roberto do Carmo Jr. |
|
|
Topographic learning and the arbitrary, categorial lexicon |
|
|
432 | (2) |
|
|
Social interaction is critical to the evolution of human communication systems (and why) |
|
|
434 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Searching for substance in the Baldwin Effect: When learning causally affects genetic adaptation |
|
|
436 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Molecular windows into speech and language |
|
|
438 | (2) |
|
|
Me to we: Cooperation, conflict, and the evolution of language |
|
|
440 | (2) |
|
|
A search for metacognition in avians |
|
|
442 | (2) |
|
|
Simulating the coevolution of language and joint attention |
|
|
444 | (2) |
|
|
|
Neutral models for language evolution |
|
|
446 | (2) |
|
|
Paleogenomics and the vocal auditory channel in archaic humans |
|
|
448 | (2) |
|
|
Infants' own-cry perception for language acquisition |
|
|
450 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Evolution of symbolic communication in the perceptual crossing experiment |
|
|
452 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Recent archaeological evidence suggests much earlier emergence of human UG |
|
|
454 | (2) |
|
|
Sound symbolism helps infants' word learning |
|
|
456 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Male gibbons change the note order according to the behavioral situations |
|
|
458 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Triadic niche construction: A scenario of human brain evolution realizing tool-use and language |
|
|
460 | (2) |
|
|
Phase theory and the role of cognitive constraints in the maximal expansion of syntactic trees |
|
|
462 | (2) |
|
|
Modelling the evolution of Creoles |
|
|
464 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Innate template difference could induce signal complexity: A comparison of song features under auditory isolation between wild and domesticated finches |
|
|
466 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
The role of STS in mimetic-word processing: Sound symbolism or biological motion? |
|
|
468 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
A parsimonious representation with hidden states for birdsong syntax: An implication to human syntax evolution |
|
|
470 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Expression analysis of language-related genes in the common marmoset brain |
|
|
472 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Song memory including sequential information in male Bengalese finches auditory area |
|
|
474 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Pitch intervals in relation to the origin of language |
|
|
476 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Minimization of Universal Grammar, reliance on third factor principles, and feasibility of inquiry into evolutionary origins |
|
|
478 | (2) |
|
|
Why language has structure: New evidence from studying cultural evolution in the lab and what it means for biological evolution |
|
|
480 | (2) |
|
|
How is pragmatic grounding formed in symbolic communication systems? |
|
|
482 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The evolution of morphological agreement |
|
|
484 | (2) |
|
|
Re-dating the loss of laryngeal air sacs in Homo sapiens |
|
|
486 | (2) |
|
|
On the origin of the hierarchy of color names |
|
|
488 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
The meaning of nonsense words |
|
|
490 | (2) |
|
|
|
Nonhuman primates do declare! Declaratives as evidence for mental time-travel in apes |
|
|
492 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Building a language-competent species: Contributions of brain and environment to cognition and communication in apes and monkeys |
|
|
494 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Dynamic expression of cadherins controls vocal learning |
|
|
496 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Simulating the effects of cultural coadaptation on rates of endogenous and exogenous language change |
|
|
498 | (2) |
|
|
The Drosophila FoxP gene is required for operant self-learning: Implications for the evolution of language |
|
|
500 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The evolution of miscommunication |
|
|
502 | (2) |
|
|
The Gricean intentional structure of ape gestural communication |
|
|
504 | (2) |
|
|
Cerebral laterality for prosody processing in human neonates: Evidence from multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy |
|
|
506 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
The irreducible semantic communicative drive: Imagination and culture beyond the hands |
|
|
508 | (2) |
|
|
Structural reanalysis of responses to musical tonality: Commonality with the neural processing of emotion in language |
|
|
510 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Failure of operant discrimination learning of simple algebraic concept in a songbird |
|
|
512 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Rodent ultrasonic vocalization as a model of human speech: A molecular and cellular view |
|
|
514 | (2) |
|
|
Parrots as models for language evolution |
|
|
516 | (2) |
|
|
A model of the evolution of frequent social communication |
|
|
518 | (2) |
|
|
Recursivity is a by-product of associative learning and working memory constraints: Evidence from baboons (Papio papio) |
|
|
520 | (3) |
|
|
|
|
A bottom-up approach to language evolution |
|
|
523 | (2) |
|
|
The nature of coordination: Evidence from Russian |
|
|
525 | (2) |
|
|
|
No words without syntax no syntax without words |
|
|
527 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Out of the brains of babes: Domain-general learning mechanisms and domain-specific systems |
|
|
529 | (4) |
|
|
Evolution of `small-world' birdsong syntax: An implication for language evolution |
|
|
533 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
How do communication systems emerge, and what does this tell us about language? |
|
|
535 | (2) |
|
|
The origin of the learning system of sequential vocalizations: Neural activity of basal-ganglia in singing behavior and a cognitive task in songbirds |
|
|
537 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Simulating language convergence |
|
|
539 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Linguistic replicators: A wild goose chase? |
|
|
541 | (2) |
|
|
Iterated learning in populations: Learning and evolving expectations about linguistic homogeneity |
|
|
543 | (2) |
|
|
|
Regularization of linguistic variation in populations |
|
|
545 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Abductive inference and insight in the evolution of symbolic communication |
|
|
547 | (2) |
|
|
Grammaticalization, light verbs, and the origins of complex language |
|
|
549 | (2) |
|
|
Divergence and convergence in vocal cultures |
|
|
551 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Replication and emergence processes in language evolution |
|
|
553 | (2) |
|
|
The effects of generation turnover and interlocutor negotiation on linguistic structure |
|
|
555 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Cultural evolution renders linguistic nativism implausible |
|
|
557 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Naming a structured world: A cultural route to duality of patterning |
|
|
559 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Neural correlates of song perception during Zebra finch song learning as shown by bold fMRI |
|
|
561 | (2) |
|
|
|
Human non-lexical vocal events and prosody in the perspective of language evolution |
|
|
563 | (2) |
|
|
|
Biased copying in the evolution of human communication systems |
|
|
565 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Emotion, approach-avoidance motivation, and blending in the evolution of language |
|
|
567 | (2) |
|
|
Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk |
|
|
The myth surrounding the ban by Societe de Linguistique de Paris |
|
|
569 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Why language evolved only once |
|
|
571 | (3) |
|
|
Intersubjectivity as the key precondition for language: Its Evolution (and possible devolution) |
|
|
574 | (3) |
|
Author Index |
|
577 | |