|
|
|
|
3 | (18) |
|
The Interdisciplinary Summer Seminar in Psychology and Linguistics and its follow-up |
|
|
3 | (1) |
|
|
3 | (4) |
|
|
7 | (4) |
|
George Miller's Language and communication |
|
|
11 | (4) |
|
Karl Lashley's The problem of serial order in behavior |
|
|
15 | (3) |
|
|
18 | (3) |
|
Part 2 Establishing the discipline: 1770-1900 |
|
|
|
2 Inventing a psychology of language |
|
|
21 | (31) |
|
|
21 | (1) |
|
From the Enlightenment to Romanticism |
|
|
21 | (4) |
|
Historical and comparative philology |
|
|
25 | (3) |
|
The root barrier and beyond |
|
|
28 | (7) |
|
Darwinian evolution and language origins |
|
|
35 | (5) |
|
|
40 | (2) |
|
|
42 | (3) |
|
|
45 | (3) |
|
Disorders of language and speech |
|
|
48 | (2) |
|
|
50 | (1) |
|
|
50 | (2) |
|
3 From bumps to diagrams: Tracing language in the brain |
|
|
52 | (41) |
|
The physiognomic beginnings |
|
|
52 | (6) |
|
|
58 | (1) |
|
|
59 | (1) |
|
The left hemisphere and Broca's area |
|
|
60 | (7) |
|
Trousseau seeds confusion |
|
|
67 | (1) |
|
Carl Wernicke and the diagram makers |
|
|
68 | (1) |
|
|
68 | (1) |
|
|
69 | (5) |
|
The Wernicke-Lichtheim model |
|
|
74 | (5) |
|
Diagram makers and making diagrams |
|
|
79 | (3) |
|
Adolf Kussmaul's textbook |
|
|
82 | (5) |
|
One more diagram maker: Jean-Martin Charcot |
|
|
87 | (1) |
|
Some non-localizationist sounds |
|
|
88 | (4) |
|
|
92 | (1) |
|
4 Language acquisition and the diary explosion |
|
|
93 | (32) |
|
Perspectives on language acquisition |
|
|
93 | (1) |
|
Early scholars of language acquisition |
|
|
94 | (1) |
|
|
94 | (1) |
|
Dietrich Tiedemann and Moritz von Winterfeld |
|
|
95 | (1) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
Hippolyte Taine and Charles Darwin |
|
|
97 | (2) |
|
Jan Baudouin de Courtenay |
|
|
99 | (1) |
|
|
100 | (1) |
|
|
100 | (1) |
|
|
100 | (1) |
|
|
101 | (3) |
|
|
104 | (1) |
|
Gabriel Compayre and Gabriel Deville |
|
|
104 | (1) |
|
|
105 | (1) |
|
|
106 | (1) |
|
|
106 | (1) |
|
|
107 | (1) |
|
The community of child language researchers |
|
|
108 | (1) |
|
Issues and controversies in child language |
|
|
108 | (1) |
|
Origins of child language |
|
|
108 | (2) |
|
|
110 | (2) |
|
|
112 | (3) |
|
Ontogenesis recapitulating phylogenesis |
|
|
115 | (1) |
|
Gestures and gesture languages |
|
|
116 | (1) |
|
Charles-Michel de l'Epee and Joseph-Marie Degerando |
|
|
117 | (4) |
|
The demise of Deaf sign language |
|
|
121 | (2) |
|
|
123 | (2) |
|
5 Language in the laboratory and modeling microgenesis |
|
|
125 | (40) |
|
Mental chronometry: Franciscus Donders |
|
|
125 | (4) |
|
Phonetics and Wolfgang von Kempelen's speaking machine |
|
|
129 | (2) |
|
|
131 | (1) |
|
|
131 | (1) |
|
|
132 | (3) |
|
Benno Erdmann and Raymond Dodge |
|
|
135 | (4) |
|
Walter Pillsbury and Oscar Quanz |
|
|
139 | (1) |
|
|
140 | (1) |
|
Speech perception and William Bagley |
|
|
141 | (3) |
|
Verbal learning, memory, and habits |
|
|
144 | (1) |
|
|
144 | (2) |
|
|
146 | (2) |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
149 | (2) |
|
Joseph Jastrow and Gustav Aschaffenburg |
|
|
151 | (1) |
|
Albert Thumb and Karl Marbe |
|
|
151 | (4) |
|
|
155 | (1) |
|
Rudolf Meringer and Carl Mayer |
|
|
155 | (8) |
|
|
163 | (1) |
|
|
164 | (1) |
|
6 Wilhelm Wundt's grand synthesis |
|
|
165 | (48) |
|
|
165 | (4) |
|
|
169 | (1) |
|
Experimental and ethnic psychology |
|
|
169 | (1) |
|
Association and apperception |
|
|
170 | (1) |
|
|
171 | (1) |
|
|
172 | (2) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
Pointing, imitating, and abstract signs |
|
|
175 | (1) |
|
Grammatical categories and sign syntax |
|
|
175 | (2) |
|
No match to spoken languages, but a window on the origins of language |
|
|
177 | (1) |
|
|
178 | (1) |
|
Evolution of vocal expression |
|
|
178 | (1) |
|
Children's acquisition of sound patterns |
|
|
179 | (1) |
|
|
180 | (1) |
|
Folk psychology of sound change |
|
|
181 | (1) |
|
Three types of sound change in the individual and in the language community |
|
|
181 | (1) |
|
Contact effects: assimilation and dissimilation |
|
|
182 | (1) |
|
Distance effects: analogy |
|
|
183 | (1) |
|
Regular sound change: Grimm's laws |
|
|
184 | (2) |
|
|
186 | (1) |
|
Word formation in brain and mind |
|
|
186 | (4) |
|
|
190 | (1) |
|
|
191 | (2) |
|
|
193 | (1) |
|
Where do sentences come from? |
|
|
193 | (2) |
|
Varieties of syntax and phrase structure |
|
|
195 | (4) |
|
|
199 | (2) |
|
Outer and internal speech form |
|
|
201 | (1) |
|
|
202 | (2) |
|
Wundt's psycholinguistic legacy |
|
|
204 | (4) |
|
Epilogue: turning the century |
|
|
208 | (5) |
|
Part 3 Twentieth-century psycholinguistics before the "cognitive revolution" |
|
|
|
7 New perspectives: Structuralism and the psychology of imageless thought |
|
|
213 | (26) |
|
Emerging structuralism: Taine, Baudouin de Courtenay, and Saussure |
|
|
213 | (5) |
|
Structuralism and the psychology of language: Sechehaye |
|
|
218 | (4) |
|
Parisian structuralism and Henri Delacroix |
|
|
222 | (3) |
|
The psychology of imageless thought: the Wurzburg school |
|
|
225 | (3) |
|
|
228 | (5) |
|
Otto Selz and Charlotte Buhler on sentence formulation |
|
|
233 | (1) |
|
|
233 | (4) |
|
|
237 | (1) |
|
|
238 | (1) |
|
|
239 | (42) |
|
Heterogeneous behaviorism |
|
|
240 | (2) |
|
Watson and vocalic thought |
|
|
242 | (3) |
|
Speech for social control: Grace de Laguna and John Markey |
|
|
245 | (4) |
|
From Stumpf to Bloomfield |
|
|
249 | (1) |
|
|
249 | (2) |
|
|
251 | (3) |
|
|
254 | (4) |
|
Bloomfield's behaviorist heritage: Zellig Harris and Noam Chomsky |
|
|
258 | (2) |
|
Kantor's psycholinguistics |
|
|
260 | (4) |
|
|
264 | (7) |
|
|
271 | (1) |
|
|
271 | (1) |
|
Cofer and Foley's analysis |
|
|
272 | (2) |
|
Charles Osgood's theory and measurement of meaning |
|
|
274 | (4) |
|
Hobart Mowrer: the sentence as conditioning device |
|
|
278 | (1) |
|
|
279 | (2) |
|
9 Speech acts and functions |
|
|
281 | (28) |
|
Philip Wegener and Adolf Reinach, the pioneers |
|
|
282 | (3) |
|
Alan Gardiner: the functions of word and sentence |
|
|
285 | (2) |
|
|
287 | (1) |
|
|
287 | (3) |
|
The functions of language |
|
|
290 | (2) |
|
|
292 | (3) |
|
The two-field theory of reference |
|
|
295 | (1) |
|
|
295 | (3) |
|
The symbol field: a two-class system |
|
|
298 | (1) |
|
The principle of abstractive reference |
|
|
298 | (1) |
|
|
299 | (1) |
|
|
300 | (1) |
|
|
301 | (1) |
|
|
302 | (2) |
|
|
304 | (1) |
|
|
305 | (1) |
|
Buhler and the Prague school |
|
|
305 | (2) |
|
Functions and speech acts in retrospect |
|
|
307 | (2) |
|
10 Language acquisition: Wealth of data, dearth of theory |
|
|
309 | (55) |
|
|
309 | (7) |
|
Leading twentieth-century scholars and research teams before the "cognitive revolution" |
|
|
316 | (1) |
|
|
316 | (1) |
|
Ivan Gheorgov and studies of self-reference |
|
|
317 | (1) |
|
Jules Ronjat and Milivoie Pavlovitch |
|
|
318 | (1) |
|
Scandinavian diary studies: Otto Jespersen |
|
|
319 | (4) |
|
Jacques van Ginneken 321 Emil FrOschels |
|
|
323 | (1) |
|
|
324 | (3) |
|
|
327 | (2) |
|
|
329 | (1) |
|
|
330 | (1) |
|
|
331 | (1) |
|
|
332 | (1) |
|
The Institutes of Child Welfare |
|
|
332 | (2) |
|
Michael Morris Lewis and his sources |
|
|
334 | (1) |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
|
335 | (2) |
|
Aleksandr Gvozdev and Werner Leopold |
|
|
337 | (1) |
|
The growth of vocabulary and utterance complexity |
|
|
338 | (5) |
|
Studies in speech sound development |
|
|
343 | (1) |
|
From first cries to words: Lewis, Buhler, and Hetzer |
|
|
344 | (3) |
|
Physiology, environment, and heredity in early sound formation: Gregoire and van Ginneken |
|
|
347 | (2) |
|
Sound assimilation and children's early words: Rottger's dissertation |
|
|
349 | (3) |
|
Jakobson on universals of phonological development |
|
|
352 | (2) |
|
The Child Welfare Institutes on early sound development |
|
|
354 | (2) |
|
Sound development in Gvozdev's and Leopold's diaries |
|
|
356 | (1) |
|
Language acquisition in bilingual environments |
|
|
357 | (3) |
|
Retrospect: data, theory, and method |
|
|
360 | (4) |
|
11 Language in the brain: The lures of holism |
|
|
364 | (52) |
|
|
364 | (1) |
|
|
365 | (1) |
|
Pierre Marie's "deconstruction" |
|
|
366 | (5) |
|
|
371 | (2) |
|
|
373 | (3) |
|
A German response: Hugo Liepmann |
|
|
376 | (2) |
|
The continuing German tradition |
|
|
378 | (1) |
|
|
378 | (2) |
|
|
380 | (2) |
|
|
382 | (1) |
|
A psychological approach to agrammatism: Arnold Pick |
|
|
383 | (4) |
|
|
387 | (1) |
|
|
387 | (2) |
|
Max Isserlin's adaptation theory |
|
|
389 | (3) |
|
Henry Head: a holist's view on theory in aphasiology |
|
|
392 | (2) |
|
|
394 | (1) |
|
Centers and their lesions |
|
|
394 | (1) |
|
|
395 | (1) |
|
|
396 | (1) |
|
|
396 | (1) |
|
|
396 | (1) |
|
Kurt Goldstein and the single case study |
|
|
397 | (1) |
|
Holism and the organismic approach |
|
|
398 | (2) |
|
General effects of brain damage |
|
|
400 | (1) |
|
Instrumentalities and abstract language |
|
|
400 | (1) |
|
|
401 | (1) |
|
|
401 | (1) |
|
Forms of language disturbance |
|
|
401 | (1) |
|
|
402 | (1) |
|
|
402 | (1) |
|
|
403 | (2) |
|
Theodore Weisenburg and Katherine McBride: aphasia is diverse |
|
|
405 | (1) |
|
Other American contributions |
|
|
406 | (2) |
|
Alexander Romanovich Luria |
|
|
408 | (2) |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
The structure of speech activity |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
|
411 | (1) |
|
|
412 | (1) |
|
|
413 | (1) |
|
Parieto-occipital systems |
|
|
413 | (1) |
|
|
414 | (2) |
|
12 Empirical studies of speech and language usage |
|
|
416 | (58) |
|
Perception and production of speech and language |
|
|
417 | (1) |
|
Perceiving consonants and vowels |
|
|
417 | (5) |
|
Harvey Fletcher's approach to intelligibility |
|
|
422 | (1) |
|
Perceiving words: noise and number of alternatives |
|
|
423 | (3) |
|
Skinner's "verbal summator" and response bias |
|
|
426 | (1) |
|
|
427 | (1) |
|
|
428 | (1) |
|
|
429 | (1) |
|
|
430 | (1) |
|
|
430 | (6) |
|
|
436 | (1) |
|
|
437 | (1) |
|
|
437 | (1) |
|
|
438 | (6) |
|
|
444 | (1) |
|
Verbal learning and memory: orders of approximation |
|
|
445 | (4) |
|
|
449 | (1) |
|
The rank-frequency distribution |
|
|
450 | (1) |
|
The number-of-words-frequency distribution: Zipfs law |
|
|
451 | (4) |
|
Zipfs law in associations |
|
|
455 | (1) |
|
Diversity of words in language usage |
|
|
455 | (1) |
|
Yule on the statistics of style |
|
|
456 | (1) |
|
Word frequency and recognition threshold |
|
|
457 | (2) |
|
Word frequency and word association |
|
|
459 | (1) |
|
Transitional probabilities |
|
|
459 | (1) |
|
|
460 | (1) |
|
|
460 | (1) |
|
|
461 | (1) |
|
|
461 | (3) |
|
|
464 | (1) |
|
|
464 | (2) |
|
|
466 | (3) |
|
|
469 | (2) |
|
|
471 | (2) |
|
|
473 | (1) |
|
13 A new cross-linguistic perspective and linguistic relativity |
|
|
474 | (34) |
|
|
475 | (3) |
|
|
478 | (1) |
|
|
478 | (1) |
|
|
479 | (3) |
|
Edward Sapir and linguistic relativism |
|
|
482 | (4) |
|
The world view approach and linguistic relativism |
|
|
486 | (1) |
|
|
486 | (2) |
|
Benjamin Whorf, self-taught linguist |
|
|
488 | (1) |
|
|
489 | (1) |
|
Whorf on linguistic relativism |
|
|
490 | (2) |
|
|
492 | (1) |
|
Whorf and the public interest |
|
|
493 | (1) |
|
|
494 | (1) |
|
|
494 | (1) |
|
|
494 | (1) |
|
|
495 | (2) |
|
|
497 | (1) |
|
|
498 | (1) |
|
Studies of relativity after Sapir-Whorf |
|
|
499 | (1) |
|
The 1953 Conference on Language in Culture |
|
|
499 | (1) |
|
The codability experiments: Eric Lenneberg and Roger Brown |
|
|
500 | (3) |
|
The coding of facial expressions |
|
|
503 | (1) |
|
Grammatical categories and cognition |
|
|
504 | (2) |
|
Retrospect: John Carroll's verdict |
|
|
506 | (2) |
|
14 Psychology of language during the Third Reich |
|
|
508 | (14) |
|
Language, race, and world view |
|
|
508 | (4) |
|
The 1931 Hamburg Congress of the German Psychological Society |
|
|
512 | (1) |
|
The 1933 Leipzig Congress of the German Psychological Society |
|
|
513 | (3) |
|
The 1933 "restoration" of the universities |
|
|
516 | (1) |
|
|
516 | (1) |
|
|
517 | (1) |
|
|
518 | (1) |
|
Kurt Goldstein and Adhemar Gelb |
|
|
518 | (1) |
|
|
519 | (1) |
|
|
520 | (1) |
|
|
521 | (1) |
|
|
521 | (1) |
|
1933-1938: some further developments |
|
|
522 | (27) |
|
|
524 | (1) |
|
|
524 | (6) |
|
|
530 | (1) |
|
|
530 | (1) |
|
|
530 | (1) |
|
|
531 | (1) |
|
German neurologists in war time |
|
|
531 | (2) |
|
|
533 | (12) |
|
|
545 | (4) |
|
Part 4 Psycholinguistics re-established |
|
|
|
15 Psycholinguistics post-war, pre-Chomsky |
|
|
549 | (28) |
|
The 1950 Conference on Speech Communication |
|
|
550 | (3) |
|
|
553 | (4) |
|
Some further developments in the study of the brain and language |
|
|
557 | (1) |
|
|
557 | (1) |
|
|
557 | (1) |
|
|
558 | (1) |
|
|
559 | (1) |
|
|
560 | (1) |
|
|
561 | (1) |
|
Canada: Wilder Penfield and electrical brain stimulation |
|
|
561 | (3) |
|
Geza Revesz and the Amsterdam symposium on thinking and speaking |
|
|
564 | (1) |
|
Old and new in developmental psycholinguistics |
|
|
565 | (1) |
|
Second-language learning and bilingualism |
|
|
566 | (2) |
|
Experimental studies of language acquisition |
|
|
568 | (3) |
|
The state of general psycholinguistics since 1951 |
|
|
571 | (6) |
Epilogue |
|
577 | (2) |
Bibliography |
|
579 | (44) |
Author Index |
|
623 | (12) |
Subject Index |
|
635 | |