Preface |
|
ix | |
|
|
xi | |
|
|
1 | (26) |
|
|
1 | (1) |
|
What is Universal Grammar a theory of? |
|
|
1 | (5) |
|
Some simple data: Georgian laterals |
|
|
6 | (1) |
|
Some more simple data: Catalan consonant alternations |
|
|
7 | (2) |
|
Traditional arguments for generativity in Phonology |
|
|
9 | (2) |
|
Does it matter which rules and representations we posit? |
|
|
11 | (11) |
|
|
22 | (5) |
|
Part I Phonological UG and acquistion |
|
|
|
The Subset Principle in Phonology |
|
|
27 | (31) |
|
|
27 | (1) |
|
The Subset Principle in learnability theory |
|
|
28 | (3) |
|
|
31 | (8) |
|
Acquisition of phonological inventories: the standard view |
|
|
39 | (3) |
|
The Subset Principle and features |
|
|
42 | (5) |
|
SP and segment ``inventories'' |
|
|
47 | (7) |
|
Innateness and maturation |
|
|
54 | (2) |
|
|
56 | (2) |
|
Competence and performance in phonological acquisition |
|
|
58 | (37) |
|
What is ``child phonology''? |
|
|
58 | (10) |
|
Optimality Theory and the competence/performance ``dilemma'' |
|
|
68 | (14) |
|
The nature of phonological acquisition |
|
|
82 | (7) |
|
|
89 | (2) |
|
|
91 | (4) |
|
The Georgian problem revisited |
|
|
95 | (10) |
|
What we now know about learning |
|
|
95 | (1) |
|
The solution to the Georgian problem |
|
|
95 | (4) |
|
English ``overgeneralization'' |
|
|
99 | (2) |
|
|
101 | (4) |
|
Part II Resisting substance abuse in phonology |
|
|
|
Isolability and idealization |
|
|
105 | (39) |
|
|
105 | (14) |
|
The ``gradedness'' of linguistic objects |
|
|
119 | (20) |
|
``Gradedness'' of grammaticality judgements |
|
|
139 | (3) |
|
|
142 | (2) |
|
Against articulatory grounding |
|
|
144 | (18) |
|
|
144 | (1) |
|
A sketch of Marshallese phonetics and phonology |
|
|
144 | (5) |
|
The phonetics-phonology interface |
|
|
149 | (5) |
|
An unsavory thought experiment |
|
|
154 | (3) |
|
The (seeming) importance of phonetics |
|
|
157 | (1) |
|
Resolving the dilemma: historical phonology |
|
|
158 | (1) |
|
A final note on ``traditionalist'' OT |
|
|
159 | (1) |
|
The irrelevance of the past |
|
|
160 | (2) |
|
Against typological grounding |
|
|
162 | (29) |
|
The irrleveance of segment markedness |
|
|
162 | (1) |
|
Form and substance in phonology |
|
|
162 | (2) |
|
Three examples of substance abuse in grammatical theory |
|
|
164 | (5) |
|
|
169 | (7) |
|
|
176 | (1) |
|
|
177 | (6) |
|
The mirage of enhancement |
|
|
183 | (1) |
|
Functionalism and dysfunctionalism |
|
|
184 | (2) |
|
|
186 | (5) |
|
Part III Some aspects of Optimality Theory |
|
|
|
|
191 | (30) |
|
|
191 | (1) |
|
The universal NoBanana constraint |
|
|
192 | (3) |
|
|
195 | (13) |
|
A right-minded approach to syntax |
|
|
208 | (1) |
|
Constraints in rule-based phonology |
|
|
209 | (2) |
|
The Obligatory Contour Principle |
|
|
211 | (5) |
|
Constraints alone vs. Rules and Constraints vs. Rules alone |
|
|
216 | (5) |
|
Against Output-Output Correspondence |
|
|
221 | (36) |
|
|
221 | (14) |
|
Other uses of Output-Output Correspondence |
|
|
235 | (18) |
|
|
253 | (4) |
|
|
|
A principled solution to Catalan |
|
|
257 | (20) |
|
Further observations on the computational machinery of phonology |
|
|
257 | (2) |
|
Feature-counting evaluation metrics |
|
|
259 | (2) |
|
Subsumption and structural descriptions---a problem |
|
|
261 | (3) |
|
|
264 | (2) |
|
The Unified Interpretive Procedure |
|
|
266 | (5) |
|
|
271 | (6) |
Final remarks |
|
277 | (2) |
References |
|
279 | (10) |
Index |
|
289 | |