List of Contributors |
|
xxi | |
Preface |
|
xxiii | |
Chapter 1 The Science of Psychology |
|
1 | (50) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
2 | (3) |
|
Basic and applied research |
|
|
3 | (2) |
|
Psychology's Roots: The Path to a Science of Mind and Behaviour |
|
|
5 | (8) |
|
|
6 | (1) |
|
The emergence of psychology as a science |
|
|
6 | (1) |
|
The psychodynamic perspective |
|
|
7 | (1) |
|
The behaviourist perspective |
|
|
8 | (1) |
|
The humanistic perspective |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
The cognitive perspective |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
The biological perspective |
|
|
10 | (1) |
|
The sociocultural perspective |
|
|
11 | (2) |
|
Levels of Analysis: Types of Psychology and Their Contribution to Understanding |
|
|
13 | (3) |
|
Understanding social anxiety using the levels of analysis framework |
|
|
13 | (3) |
|
Issues and Debates in Psychology |
|
|
16 | (6) |
|
|
16 | (1) |
|
Nature-nurture debate (heredity versus environment) |
|
|
17 | (1) |
|
|
18 | (1) |
|
Free will versus determinism |
|
|
19 | (1) |
|
|
20 | (2) |
|
Bias in Psychological Research |
|
|
22 | (3) |
|
|
22 | (2) |
|
Tackling bias in psychology |
|
|
24 | (1) |
|
Psychology at the Cultural Level |
|
|
25 | (4) |
|
|
25 | (1) |
|
Cross-cultural psychology |
|
|
26 | (1) |
|
Approaches: Etic-emic distinction |
|
|
26 | (1) |
|
Individualism versus collectivism |
|
|
26 | (1) |
|
Difficulties researching within cross-cultural psychology |
|
|
27 | (1) |
|
Examples of cross-cultural differences in psychological research |
|
|
27 | (2) |
|
|
29 | (3) |
|
Psychology as an academic subject |
|
|
29 | (1) |
|
Governing and professional bodies in psychology |
|
|
30 | (2) |
|
Psychology in Action: Applying Psychological Science |
|
|
32 | (8) |
|
The psychological professions |
|
|
34 | (2) |
|
How do I become a professional psychologist? |
|
|
36 | (2) |
|
What if these careers don't appeal? What else can I do with a psychology degree? |
|
|
38 | (2) |
|
|
40 | (2) |
|
Future developments in academic psychology and research |
|
|
40 | (1) |
|
Future developments in the psychology professions and the impact on society |
|
|
40 | (2) |
|
|
42 | (9) |
Chapter 2 Research Methods in Psychology |
|
51 | (56) |
|
|
|
52 | (3) |
|
An example of a scientific question |
|
|
53 | (2) |
|
The Dissemination of Scientific Ideas |
|
|
55 | (1) |
|
Quantitative Research Methods |
|
|
56 | (21) |
|
What constitutes 'good' research? Reliability, validity, and importance |
|
|
57 | (1) |
|
|
58 | (1) |
|
|
59 | (1) |
|
Methods for studying behaviour |
|
|
60 | (5) |
|
How to produce a well-designed experiment |
|
|
65 | (1) |
|
More complex experimental designs |
|
|
66 | (11) |
|
Analysing and Interpreting Quantitative Data |
|
|
77 | (5) |
|
|
77 | (1) |
|
|
78 | (1) |
|
Null hypothesis significance testing |
|
|
78 | (2) |
|
Statistical significance and effect size |
|
|
80 | (2) |
|
Qualitative Research Methods |
|
|
82 | (5) |
|
Methods for obtaining qualitative data |
|
|
83 | (1) |
|
Qualitative data analysis techniques |
|
|
83 | (2) |
|
Strengths of qualitative approaches |
|
|
85 | (1) |
|
Weaknesses of qualitative approaches |
|
|
86 | (1) |
|
Good and Bad Practice in Psychological Research |
|
|
87 | (8) |
|
Ethical principles in research |
|
|
88 | (3) |
|
|
91 | (2) |
|
|
93 | (2) |
|
|
95 | (13) |
|
Brain imaging versus behavioural measures |
|
|
95 | (1) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
The demise of null hypothesis significance testing? |
|
|
97 | (1) |
|
|
98 | (9) |
Chapter 3 Evolutionary and Genetic Foundations of Psychology |
|
107 | (50) |
|
|
|
108 | (4) |
|
What Makes You Unique? The Origins of Variation |
|
|
112 | (8) |
|
|
113 | (2) |
|
|
115 | (1) |
|
|
116 | (4) |
|
Passing It On: The Mechanisms of Heredity |
|
|
120 | (9) |
|
|
121 | (2) |
|
Genetically complex traits |
|
|
123 | (1) |
|
|
124 | (2) |
|
Hunting for the genetic basis of psychological characteristics |
|
|
126 | (3) |
|
Staying Alive: The Operation of Natural Selection |
|
|
129 | (5) |
|
Consequences of differential reproductive success |
|
|
129 | (2) |
|
Selection at the genetic level |
|
|
131 | (3) |
|
Staying Alive: Selection of Behaviours and Adaptation |
|
|
134 | (4) |
|
|
134 | (3) |
|
Adaptation: The consequence of natural selection |
|
|
137 | (1) |
|
The Value of Evolution for Psychology |
|
|
138 | (9) |
|
Negative emotions and judgement biases |
|
|
138 | (2) |
|
|
140 | (2) |
|
Learning and the lifecycle in humans and chimpanzees |
|
|
142 | (5) |
|
|
147 | (11) |
|
|
147 | (1) |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
149 | (1) |
|
|
150 | (7) |
Chapter 4 Neuroscience, the Brain, and Behaviour |
|
157 | (52) |
|
|
|
|
158 | (1) |
|
Neurons and Synaptic Transmission |
|
|
159 | (14) |
|
|
160 | (1) |
|
Electrical activity of neurons |
|
|
160 | (1) |
|
|
161 | (1) |
|
GABA and glutamate: Stop and go |
|
|
162 | (2) |
|
Modulators: Acetylcholine, serotonin, dopamine, and noradrenaline |
|
|
164 | (2) |
|
Changes in synaptic connectivity as the basis of learning and memory: 'Cells that fire together wire together' |
|
|
166 | (4) |
|
How neurons code information |
|
|
170 | (3) |
|
An Overview of Brain Structures and Their Functions |
|
|
173 | (6) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
General architecture of the brain |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
Functions of the cortical lobes of the brain |
|
|
175 | (1) |
|
Subcortical systems and the midbrain and hindbrain |
|
|
176 | (3) |
|
Animal Neuroscience Methods |
|
|
179 | (5) |
|
|
180 | (1) |
|
|
181 | (1) |
|
In vivo electrophysiology, microinjection, or microdialysis |
|
|
181 | (1) |
|
Genetic basis of behaviour |
|
|
182 | (2) |
|
Human Neuroscience Methods |
|
|
184 | (8) |
|
Electrical recording methods |
|
|
185 | (2) |
|
|
187 | (1) |
|
|
188 | (2) |
|
|
190 | (1) |
|
|
191 | (1) |
|
Hemispheric Lateralization |
|
|
192 | (4) |
|
|
196 | (3) |
|
|
199 | (1) |
|
|
200 | (9) |
Chapter 5 Motivation |
|
209 | (53) |
|
|
|
210 | (9) |
|
|
210 | (1) |
|
|
211 | (4) |
|
|
215 | (1) |
|
|
216 | (1) |
|
|
217 | (2) |
|
The Biological Bases of Primary Motivations: Hunger and Drinking |
|
|
219 | (10) |
|
|
219 | (7) |
|
|
226 | (3) |
|
The Biological Bases of Primary Motivations: Sex and Aggression |
|
|
229 | (11) |
|
|
230 | (6) |
|
|
236 | (4) |
|
Intrinsic Versus Extrinsic Motivation |
|
|
240 | (3) |
|
Long-term goals and how to stay focused |
|
|
241 | (1) |
|
Self-determination theory |
|
|
242 | (1) |
|
Affiliation and Achievement Motivation |
|
|
243 | (4) |
|
|
244 | (1) |
|
|
245 | (2) |
|
Applications of Theories of Motivation |
|
|
247 | (16) |
|
|
248 | (1) |
|
|
248 | (1) |
|
|
249 | (1) |
|
|
250 | (12) |
Chapter 6 Consciousness: Conscious Versus Unconscious Processes |
|
262 | (62) |
|
|
|
|
263 | (1) |
|
Different uses of the word 'conscious' |
|
|
263 | (1) |
|
Theories of Consciousness: Higher-Order Versus Integration Theories |
|
|
264 | (6) |
|
|
264 | (4) |
|
Measuring the conscious status of mental states: Measures and theories |
|
|
268 | (2) |
|
Conscious Versus Unconscious Memory |
|
|
270 | (9) |
|
|
271 | (1) |
|
|
271 | (2) |
|
|
273 | (1) |
|
General anaesthesia and sleep |
|
|
274 | (1) |
|
|
275 | (1) |
|
Motivated forgetting of episodes |
|
|
276 | (3) |
|
Conscious Versus Unconscious Perception |
|
|
279 | (13) |
|
|
279 | (2) |
|
|
281 | (3) |
|
Process dissociation, awareness, and control |
|
|
284 | (3) |
|
Neural correlates of consciousness |
|
|
287 | (3) |
|
|
290 | (2) |
|
Conscious Versus Unconscious Learning |
|
|
292 | (11) |
|
|
293 | (1) |
|
Implicit learning in the test tube: Grammars |
|
|
294 | (3) |
|
Implicit learning in the test tube: Perceptual motor skills |
|
|
297 | (3) |
|
Implicit learning and education |
|
|
300 | (3) |
|
Conscious Versus Unconscious Intentions: Volition and Hypnotic Response |
|
|
303 | (4) |
|
|
303 | (1) |
|
Ideomotor action and hypnotic response |
|
|
304 | (3) |
|
Conscious Versus Unconscious Attitudes and Emotions |
|
|
307 | (3) |
|
If you have attitude, must you know it? |
|
|
307 | (2) |
|
|
309 | (1) |
|
Functions of Consciousness and Future Directions |
|
|
310 | (2) |
|
|
311 | (1) |
|
|
312 | (12) |
Chapter 7 Sensation and Perception |
|
324 | (58) |
|
|
|
325 | (8) |
|
Approaches to research in sensation and perception |
|
|
326 | (1) |
|
|
326 | (3) |
|
|
329 | (4) |
|
The Neuroscience of Sensation and Perception |
|
|
333 | (7) |
|
|
333 | (1) |
|
|
334 | (1) |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
Stimulus tuning in sensory neurons |
|
|
336 | (1) |
|
Topographical maps in sensory processing |
|
|
337 | (3) |
|
Sensory Qualia and Modality |
|
|
340 | (7) |
|
|
342 | (1) |
|
|
343 | (1) |
|
|
344 | (1) |
|
|
345 | (2) |
|
|
347 | (7) |
|
|
348 | (2) |
|
|
350 | (1) |
|
|
350 | (2) |
|
|
352 | (2) |
|
|
354 | (6) |
|
|
354 | (1) |
|
|
354 | (1) |
|
|
355 | (2) |
|
|
357 | (2) |
|
Dorsal and ventral processing streams |
|
|
359 | (1) |
|
Vision: Perceiving Object Properties |
|
|
360 | (6) |
|
After-effects and illusions |
|
|
360 | (1) |
|
|
361 | (1) |
|
|
362 | (4) |
|
|
366 | (6) |
|
|
367 | (1) |
|
|
368 | (1) |
|
|
368 | (1) |
|
|
369 | (1) |
|
Explanations of multimodal effects in perception |
|
|
370 | (2) |
|
|
372 | (3) |
|
|
375 | (7) |
Chapter 8 Learning |
|
382 | (47) |
|
|
Introduction: What Is Learning and Why Does It Happen? |
|
|
383 | (2) |
|
|
383 | (1) |
|
|
384 | (1) |
|
|
385 | (19) |
|
Habituation and sensitization |
|
|
385 | (3) |
|
|
388 | (6) |
|
Instrumental conditioning |
|
|
394 | (5) |
|
|
399 | (5) |
|
|
404 | (3) |
|
The Theoretical Basis of Learning |
|
|
407 | (4) |
|
|
407 | (1) |
|
The Rescorla-Wagner model |
|
|
408 | (1) |
|
Successes of the Rescorla-Wagner model |
|
|
409 | (1) |
|
Limitations of the Rescorla-Wagner model |
|
|
409 | (2) |
|
The Adaptive Brain: Learning Through Connections |
|
|
411 | (2) |
|
Long-term potentiation and depression |
|
|
411 | (1) |
|
The neural basis of surprise |
|
|
412 | (1) |
|
Practical and Clinical Applications of Learning |
|
|
413 | (8) |
|
|
414 | (2) |
|
|
416 | (5) |
|
Conclusions and Future Directions |
|
|
421 | (1) |
|
|
422 | (7) |
Chapter 9 Memory |
|
429 | (50) |
|
|
|
|
430 | (6) |
|
|
430 | (1) |
|
|
431 | (1) |
|
|
432 | (2) |
|
The history of memory research |
|
|
434 | (2) |
|
Memory for Beginners: The Cognitive Psychology of Remembering |
|
|
436 | (7) |
|
Forgetting and the forgetting curve |
|
|
436 | (1) |
|
|
437 | (6) |
|
Thinking About Memory: Models and Frameworks |
|
|
443 | (6) |
|
|
443 | (3) |
|
The levels of processing framework: Memory as a process |
|
|
446 | (3) |
|
Three Dichotomies in Human Memory |
|
|
449 | (19) |
|
Implicit versus explicit memory |
|
|
449 | (4) |
|
Short-term versus long-term memory |
|
|
453 | (6) |
|
General knowledge versus personal events |
|
|
459 | (9) |
|
|
468 | (4) |
|
|
472 | (7) |
Chapter 10 Emotion |
|
479 | (50) |
|
|
|
480 | (1) |
|
|
481 | (6) |
|
What makes emotion emotional? |
|
|
481 | (2) |
|
What's different about different emotions? |
|
|
483 | (4) |
|
|
487 | (9) |
|
|
488 | (3) |
|
Autonomic feedback hypothesis |
|
|
491 | (2) |
|
Central nervous system activity |
|
|
493 | (3) |
|
|
496 | (9) |
|
|
496 | (3) |
|
Are there universal facial expressions? |
|
|
499 | (2) |
|
Are facial expressions shown consistently in emotional situations? |
|
|
501 | (1) |
|
|
502 | (3) |
|
|
505 | (10) |
|
Appraisal and emotion differentiation |
|
|
506 | (3) |
|
Appraisal as a cause of emotion |
|
|
509 | (2) |
|
|
511 | (4) |
|
What's Different About Different Emotions? |
|
|
515 | (4) |
|
|
515 | (1) |
|
Explaining response coherence |
|
|
516 | (1) |
|
|
516 | (3) |
|
Conclusions and Future Directions |
|
|
519 | (2) |
|
|
521 | (8) |
Chapter 11 Cognitive Development |
|
529 | (60) |
|
|
|
530 | (2) |
|
|
531 | (1) |
|
Theories of Development: How Nature and Nurture Interact |
|
|
532 | (6) |
|
Early theoretical accounts of development |
|
|
532 | (1) |
|
Jean Piaget's constructionism |
|
|
533 | (2) |
|
Developmental cognitive neuroscience |
|
|
535 | (3) |
|
Research Methods in Developmental Psychology |
|
|
538 | (3) |
|
Researching developmental change |
|
|
538 | (1) |
|
Working with infants and children |
|
|
539 | (2) |
|
Development of the Brain and Nervous System |
|
|
541 | (6) |
|
Biological development from conception to birth |
|
|
543 | (2) |
|
Biological development from infancy to adulthood |
|
|
545 | (2) |
|
Multisensory Perceptual Development |
|
|
547 | (9) |
|
Prenatal and newborn perceptual abilities |
|
|
548 | (4) |
|
Perceptual development in the first year of life |
|
|
552 | (2) |
|
How we come to use our senses together |
|
|
554 | (2) |
|
|
556 | (6) |
|
|
557 | (2) |
|
Learning to link perception to action |
|
|
559 | (3) |
|
|
562 | (15) |
|
The origins of knowledge in infancy |
|
|
563 | (3) |
|
Development of logical thought in childhood |
|
|
566 | (4) |
|
Development of cognitive functions: |
|
|
|
The information-processing approach |
|
|
570 | (2) |
|
Social cognitive development |
|
|
572 | (4) |
|
Cognitive development in adulthood and old age |
|
|
576 | (1) |
|
|
577 | (12) |
Chapter 12 Language and Thought |
|
589 | (61) |
|
|
|
590 | (1) |
|
|
591 | (18) |
|
Human and animal communication |
|
|
591 | (2) |
|
The evolution of language |
|
|
593 | (1) |
|
The structure of language |
|
|
594 | (2) |
|
Language acquisition by children |
|
|
596 | (2) |
|
Understanding and producing language |
|
|
598 | (3) |
|
|
601 | (3) |
|
|
604 | (1) |
|
The biological bases of language and language disorders |
|
|
605 | (2) |
|
|
607 | (2) |
|
|
609 | (30) |
|
|
610 | (2) |
|
Reasoning: Deductive and inductive |
|
|
612 | (3) |
|
|
615 | (5) |
|
Judgement and decision making |
|
|
620 | (6) |
|
|
626 | (3) |
|
The neuropsychology of thinking |
|
|
629 | (1) |
|
The development of thinking |
|
|
629 | (1) |
|
|
630 | (1) |
|
|
630 | (3) |
|
|
633 | (1) |
|
Knowledge, expertise, and wisdom |
|
|
633 | (2) |
|
|
635 | (1) |
|
|
636 | (3) |
|
|
639 | (2) |
|
|
639 | (1) |
|
The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis (linguistic determinism and linguistic relativity) |
|
|
639 | (1) |
|
|
640 | (1) |
|
|
641 | (9) |
Chapter 13 Intelligence |
|
650 | (43) |
|
|
Introduction and Defining Intelligence |
|
|
651 | (3) |
|
Difficulties in defining intelligence |
|
|
651 | (1) |
|
A consensus definition of intelligence |
|
|
652 | (1) |
|
A working definition of intelligence |
|
|
653 | (1) |
|
|
654 | (4) |
|
The influence of Francis Galton |
|
|
654 | (1) |
|
Intelligence and academic performance |
|
|
655 | (1) |
|
The first intelligence test |
|
|
656 | (1) |
|
|
657 | (1) |
|
Modern Intelligence Tests |
|
|
658 | (5) |
|
Similarities in intelligence tests |
|
|
659 | (2) |
|
Differences in intelligence tests |
|
|
661 | (2) |
|
|
663 | (8) |
|
|
664 | (2) |
|
|
666 | (1) |
|
|
667 | (1) |
|
|
668 | (1) |
|
Recent models of the structure of intelligence |
|
|
669 | (1) |
|
Critique of structural models of intelligence |
|
|
669 | (2) |
|
|
671 | (5) |
|
Intrapersonal stability and change: Fluid and crystallized intelligence |
|
|
671 | (2) |
|
Interpersonal stability and change |
|
|
673 | (2) |
|
The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 |
|
|
675 | (1) |
|
Causes of Individual Differences in Intelligence |
|
|
676 | (8) |
|
|
677 | (1) |
|
Genetic influences on intelligence |
|
|
678 | (1) |
|
Gene-environment correlations |
|
|
678 | (1) |
|
Gene-environment interactions |
|
|
679 | (1) |
|
|
680 | (1) |
|
|
681 | (1) |
|
|
682 | (2) |
|
Alternative Models of Intelligence |
|
|
684 | (3) |
|
|
684 | (1) |
|
The triarchic theory of intelligence |
|
|
685 | (1) |
|
|
686 | (1) |
|
Issues with theories of hot intelligence |
|
|
686 | (1) |
|
|
687 | (2) |
|
|
689 | (4) |
Chapter 14 Personality |
|
693 | (57) |
|
|
Introduction to Personality |
|
|
694 | (3) |
|
Humans: Individuals and types |
|
|
694 | (1) |
|
Theories, data, and science |
|
|
694 | (1) |
|
Relativity, conditionality, and statistics |
|
|
695 | (1) |
|
Description and explanation |
|
|
695 | (1) |
|
|
695 | (1) |
|
|
696 | (1) |
|
Psychodynamic Personality Theory |
|
|
697 | (11) |
|
|
697 | (1) |
|
|
697 | (1) |
|
|
698 | (1) |
|
|
698 | (3) |
|
|
701 | (1) |
|
|
701 | (1) |
|
Trauma, anxiety, and coping |
|
|
702 | (2) |
|
Human nature and variability |
|
|
704 | (1) |
|
Psychoanalytic personality research |
|
|
705 | (1) |
|
The authoritarian personality |
|
|
706 | (1) |
|
|
707 | (1) |
|
|
707 | (1) |
|
|
708 | (11) |
|
Qualities of manifest traits |
|
|
709 | (1) |
|
Organizing trait descriptions |
|
|
709 | (3) |
|
|
712 | (1) |
|
Matching measures with what they are supposed to measure |
|
|
712 | (1) |
|
|
713 | (1) |
|
|
714 | (1) |
|
|
715 | (1) |
|
The altruistic personality |
|
|
716 | (1) |
|
|
717 | (1) |
|
|
717 | (2) |
|
Humanistic Personality Theory |
|
|
719 | (11) |
|
|
719 | (1) |
|
|
720 | (1) |
|
|
720 | (1) |
|
|
721 | (1) |
|
|
721 | (1) |
|
|
722 | (1) |
|
|
723 | (1) |
|
|
724 | (3) |
|
|
727 | (1) |
|
|
728 | (2) |
|
Social Cognitive Personality Theories |
|
|
730 | (7) |
|
|
731 | (1) |
|
|
732 | (1) |
|
|
733 | (1) |
|
|
733 | (1) |
|
Goals and self-regulation |
|
|
734 | (1) |
|
|
734 | (1) |
|
|
734 | (2) |
|
|
736 | (1) |
|
Conclusions and Future Directions |
|
|
737 | (3) |
|
|
740 | (10) |
Chapter 15 Mental Health and Psychopathology |
|
750 | (53) |
|
|
|
Introduction to Psychopathology |
|
|
751 | (2) |
|
The prevalence of mental health problems |
|
|
752 | (1) |
|
A Brief History of Psychopathology |
|
|
753 | (3) |
|
|
753 | (1) |
|
The medical or disease model |
|
|
754 | (2) |
|
|
756 | (4) |
|
Deviation from the statistical norm |
|
|
757 | (2) |
|
Deviation from social and political norms |
|
|
759 | (1) |
|
|
759 | (1) |
|
Explanatory Approaches to Psychopathology |
|
|
760 | (6) |
|
|
761 | (1) |
|
|
762 | (4) |
|
Classifying Mental Health Problems |
|
|
766 | (2) |
|
The development of classification systems |
|
|
767 | (1) |
|
|
768 | (3) |
|
|
771 | (18) |
|
Anxiety and anxiety-related disorders |
|
|
772 | (4) |
|
|
776 | (3) |
|
Schizophrenia spectrum and other psychotic disorders |
|
|
779 | (6) |
|
|
785 | (4) |
|
Conclusions and Future Directions |
|
|
789 | (2) |
|
|
791 | (12) |
Chapter 16 The Treatment of Mental Health Problems |
|
803 | (52) |
|
|
|
|
804 | (5) |
|
Mental health professionals and health service provision |
|
|
806 | (3) |
|
Psychological Treatments for Mental Health Problems |
|
|
809 | (13) |
|
|
810 | (2) |
|
|
812 | (1) |
|
|
813 | (1) |
|
|
813 | (4) |
|
|
817 | (3) |
|
Systemic and family therapies |
|
|
820 | (1) |
|
Humanistic psychotherapies |
|
|
821 | (1) |
|
Ways of Delivering Psychological Therapy |
|
|
822 | (3) |
|
|
822 | (1) |
|
|
823 | (1) |
|
|
823 | (1) |
|
|
823 | (2) |
|
Social and Environmental Approaches to Treatment |
|
|
825 | (3) |
|
Social group interventions |
|
|
825 | (1) |
|
Community psychology interventions |
|
|
825 | (1) |
|
Cultural and gender issues in psychological therapies |
|
|
826 | (2) |
|
Biological Approaches to Treatment |
|
|
828 | (4) |
|
|
828 | (1) |
|
|
829 | (1) |
|
|
830 | (1) |
|
Problems with drug treatments |
|
|
830 | (1) |
|
Electroconvulsive therapy |
|
|
831 | (1) |
|
Evaluating Treatments for Mental Health Problems |
|
|
832 | (10) |
|
|
833 | (4) |
|
Do the benefits exceed placebo effects? |
|
|
837 | (1) |
|
Do patients make changes that are clinically meaningful? |
|
|
838 | (1) |
|
Do patients maintain their gains? |
|
|
839 | (1) |
|
How much therapy is necessary? |
|
|
839 | (1) |
|
Do some patients get worse? |
|
|
839 | (1) |
|
Do the treatment effects generalize to routine care settings? |
|
|
840 | (1) |
|
|
841 | (1) |
|
Qualitative research in treatment evaluation |
|
|
842 | (1) |
|
Conclusions and Future Directions |
|
|
842 | (1) |
|
|
843 | (12) |
Chapter 17 Social Development |
|
855 | (80) |
|
|
|
|
|
856 | (1) |
|
Early Emotional Development and Temperament |
|
|
857 | (6) |
|
Early emotional expressions |
|
|
858 | (2) |
|
Temperament and plasticity |
|
|
860 | (3) |
|
Caregiver-Child Interactions and the Development of Attachment |
|
|
863 | (10) |
|
Social communication in infancy |
|
|
863 | (2) |
|
|
865 | (2) |
|
Measuring the attachment relationship |
|
|
867 | (2) |
|
Antecedents of attachment |
|
|
869 | (1) |
|
Consequences of attachment |
|
|
870 | (1) |
|
Maternal deprivation and disruption to attachment |
|
|
871 | (2) |
|
Social Understanding and Self-Regulation |
|
|
873 | (10) |
|
|
874 | (4) |
|
Emotion understanding and empathy |
|
|
878 | (1) |
|
Self-regulation and executive function |
|
|
879 | (4) |
|
|
883 | (7) |
|
Distinguishing between social-conventional and moral rules |
|
|
886 | (1) |
|
|
887 | (1) |
|
|
887 | (3) |
|
Who Am I? The Development of Self and Identity |
|
|
890 | (6) |
|
|
890 | (2) |
|
|
892 | (2) |
|
Ethnic and national identity |
|
|
894 | (2) |
|
Developing in a Social Context: Family, Peers, and Media |
|
|
896 | (11) |
|
|
896 | (3) |
|
|
899 | (1) |
|
|
900 | (3) |
|
Media, television, and the internet |
|
|
903 | (4) |
|
|
907 | (1) |
|
|
908 | (27) |
Chapter 18 Social Cognition and Attitudes |
|
935 | (51) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
936 | (3) |
|
Why have humans evolved to think socially? |
|
|
937 | (1) |
|
Organizing social thinking |
|
|
938 | (1) |
|
|
939 | (5) |
|
|
939 | (1) |
|
|
940 | (1) |
|
|
941 | (1) |
|
|
942 | (2) |
|
Person Perception 1: Social Inference and Attribution |
|
|
944 | (4) |
|
|
944 | (1) |
|
|
945 | (2) |
|
|
947 | (1) |
|
Person Perception 2: Social Cognition and Social Knowledge |
|
|
948 | (4) |
|
|
948 | (2) |
|
Categorization versus individuation |
|
|
950 | (1) |
|
|
950 | (1) |
|
Disadvantages of categorical thinking |
|
|
951 | (1) |
|
|
952 | (6) |
|
Why do people stereotype? |
|
|
953 | (1) |
|
Stereotype formation and maintenance |
|
|
953 | (3) |
|
When do people stereotype? |
|
|
956 | (2) |
|
Attitudes and Attitude Change |
|
|
958 | (11) |
|
|
958 | (1) |
|
|
959 | (2) |
|
|
961 | (4) |
|
|
965 | (4) |
|
Automatic Processes in Social Cognition |
|
|
969 | (3) |
|
Automatic and controlled thinking |
|
|
969 | (1) |
|
|
970 | (2) |
|
|
972 | (2) |
|
|
974 | (12) |
Chapter 19 Interpersonal, Group, and Intergroup Processes |
|
986 | (43) |
|
|
|
987 | (1) |
|
|
987 | (11) |
|
|
988 | (3) |
|
|
991 | (3) |
|
|
994 | (4) |
|
|
998 | (9) |
|
|
998 | (1) |
|
|
999 | (3) |
|
|
1002 | (5) |
|
|
1007 | (10) |
|
|
1007 | (1) |
|
|
1008 | (3) |
|
|
1011 | (6) |
|
|
1017 | (1) |
|
|
1018 | (11) |
Index |
|
1029 | |